We Studied Who’s Most Susceptible To Racial Dog Whistles. It’s Not Who You’d Expect.

New research on the impact of racial dog whistling on Americans offers more insight on Obama-Trump swing voters.
TPM Illustration/Getty Images
Start your day with TPM.
Sign up for the Morning Memo newsletter

This article is part of TPM Cafe, TPM’s home for opinion and news analysis. 

Since Donald Trump’s surprise victory in 2016, political observers have sought to understand the elusive Obama-Trump swing voter — Americans who voted for the Democratic candidate in 2012 but who switched party loyalties in 2016 to vote for a candidate who regularly invoked negative stereotypes of racial minorities.

My latest research may shed more light onto this mysterious group of voters.

In a study on racial dog whistles, we found that a surprising group of Americans is particularly susceptible to attempts to sway their opinions through references to racial stereotypes.

Racial dog whistling has long been a tactic of Republican political messaging. However, our research suggests that in the current political environment, these messages appear to be most effective in swaying the political opinions of liberals — specifically white liberals who harbor higher than average levels of racial resentment.

In two experiments I conducted with Robb Willer of Stanford University, we examined the effects of implicit and explicit racial cues on white Americans’ evaluations of welfare programs and gun control policies. We did this by presenting them with political messages that subtly or overtly cued racial stereotypes, and then comparing their political attitudes to those of participants who read a message without any racial content.

We found that subtle racial cues did indeed influence political opinions — but most consistently among white, racially resentful liberals.

What’s even more surprising are the results of the overt racial cues.

A long tradition of research in political behavior suggests that white Americans’ political opinions can be influenced by subtle racial cues, but that these racial “dog whistles” can only influence white peoples’ attitudes if the racial content of the message remains outside of conscious awareness. Once the racial meaning becomes too obvious, the message should backfire, as most will reject it as racist.

Yet we found that the white, racially resentful liberals who responded to the subtle cues also responded most strongly to the overtly racist message, despite the fact that participants overwhelmingly acknowledged that this message was offensive.

We didn’t predict these results, which means they should be subjected to further study. In addition, we can only speculate as to why racially resentful liberals might be more likely than other Americans to respond to this rhetoric. For example, it may be that conservative news sources already discuss issues of poverty and crime in such racially loaded terms that any message we presented to conservative participants essentially echoed arguments with which they were already familiar. That would lead these appeals to have little effect on conservative participants’ opinions, leaving us to see dog-whistle effects only among liberals.

That said, these surprising findings may lend insight into Obama-Trump swing voters.

The results of our experiments suggest that racially resentful, white liberals are most swayed by attempts to tap into latent racial animosities. By analyzing national survey data, we also found that this same group was particularly likely to switch from voting for Barack Obama in 2012 to Donald Trump in 2016. While our research can’t prove that these individuals voted for Trump because his campaign messages invoked negative stereotypes of racial minorities, the pattern is suggestive.

The figure below graphs the results of our statistical analysis predicting how likely a white American is to be an Obama-Trump swing voter by their political ideology and racial attitudes. Among white liberals highest in racial resentment, the probability of being an Obama-Trump swing voter is over 50%.

Together, these findings support the idea that Donald Trump’s inflammatory racial rhetoric helped him win support among some white, traditionally Democratic voters, propelling him to the presidency in 2016.

To be clear, our findings do not suggest that liberals are the “real racists” in America, as some spin of these findings might have you believe. While we found that white liberals high in racial resentment are most responsive to this rhetoric, most liberals report relatively low levels of racial resentment. That means that a) most liberals in our studies were not swayed by racial dog whistles, and b) on average, moderates and conservatives in our studies reported higher levels of racial resentment (and other types of racial prejudice) than did liberals.

Instead, one takeaway is that this group of potential swing voters is not comprised of moderates in the traditional sense. Rather, what stands out about these racially resentful, white liberals are their conflicting political attitudes. Their racial attitudes are not aligned with their political ideologies in the same way as most white Americans. As noted above, most liberals in today’s political environment express relatively low levels of racial resentment, and most conservatives express relatively high levels of racial resentment. These racially resentful liberals, on the other hand, have liberal ideologies and typically Democratic partisan identities but are more similar to conservatives in their racial attitudes.

These conflicting attitudes appear to make this group vulnerable to attempts to influence their political opinions with subtle and overt racial appeals, a factor that may have led many of them to vote for Donald Trump.

However, their liberal ideologies and Democratic partisan identities may also mean they can be influenced by principled appeals for traditional liberal priorities, such as an expanded social safety net and government intervention to build a better society. Though this is beyond the scope of our research, this suggests that if Democrats tap into these liberal values and sense of partisan loyalty, these voters may well swing back.

 


Rachel Wetts is the Acacia Assistant Professor of Environment and Society and Sociology at Brown University. Her research focuses on the politics of white racial resentment, and the problem of stalled political action to address climate change.

Latest Cafe

Notable Replies

  1. I swear I read this article weeks ago on Huffpo

  2. What percentage of the electorate are these white, racist liberals?

  3. Despite the anger his column provoked, this is why Dana Milbank was essentially correct when he said that calling Trump a racist is counter-productive. It triggers racial resentments, and until these people die off or stop voting, it’s more effective not to trigger them. When they hear “Trump is a racist” what they process is “You’re calling me a racist because I considered voting for or did vote for, Trump.” Yes, he’s a racist. Yes, they’re also racist. But if we put the emphasis on “Trump is divisive,” “Trump is a horrible human being with cruel policies,” “Trump is corrupt, and inept, and surrounded by toadies,” etc., we’ll do a better job of reminding people why they don’t like Trump rather than why they don’t like progressives who call them out.

  4. Avatar for jep07 jep07 says:

    Brainwashed, prejudiced people don’t react with open minds, so they are dull in their immediate response. The indignation “liberals” feel comes mostly from a seething internal outrage at how casually those racist remarks are uttered and how blithely the cult members respond. To them it is just conversation, to “us” it is trash talk.

    We, of course, are right. Their behavior is reprehensible and we feel it. They just do it by rote.

    They made a habit out of hate and now it is their norm, why would they respond indignantly?

    We hear it, they live it.

    There are none so blind…

  5. Avatar for jep07 jep07 says:

    Are they talking about reactions, per se, and not necessarily racist response?

    It isn’t that they are racists, it is that they recognize the racism from a place of disapproval and react accordingly, while the racists just roll with it and have no reaction because they are, well, racists, they aren’t disgusted by it, they do it all day long among their peers.
    Maybe I read it wrong.

Continue the discussion at forums.talkingpointsmemo.com

63 more replies

Participants

Avatar for buckguy Avatar for iamtheshizznit Avatar for jep07 Avatar for rick Avatar for sniffit Avatar for tomanjeri Avatar for midnight_rambler Avatar for radgal70 Avatar for khaaannn Avatar for bill_ellis Avatar for beattycat Avatar for brewhousebob Avatar for pareedave Avatar for chlarry Avatar for sweetwilliam Avatar for oncesain Avatar for bloomingpeonie Avatar for swampsofjersey Avatar for paul_lukasiak Avatar for kovie Avatar for laparque Avatar for Chiral Avatar for Kappus Avatar for BarefootTX

Continue Discussion
Masthead Masthead
Founder & Editor-in-Chief:
Executive Editor:
Managing Editor:
Associate Editor:
Editor at Large:
General Counsel:
Publisher:
Head of Product:
Director of Technology:
Associate Publisher:
Front End Developer:
Senior Designer: